A Unified Theory of Development: A Dialectic Integration of Nature and Nurture
concerning this article should be addressed to Arnold Sameroff, Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan, 300 N. Ingalls Building, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-0406. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected].
Abstract
The understanding of nature and nurture within developmental science has evolved with alternating ascendance of one or the other as primary explanations for individual differences in life course trajectories of success or failure. A dialectical perspective emphasizing the interconnectedness of individual and context is suggested to interpret the evolution of developmental science in similar terms to those necessary to explain the development of individual children. A unified theory of development is proposed to integrate personal change, context, regulation, and representational models of development.
References
- Baltes, P. B. (1979). Life-span developmental psychology: Some converging observations on history and theory. In P. B. Baltes & O. G. Brim (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 256–281). New York: Academic Press.
-
Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Toward a model of culture-parent-child transactions. In
A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 139–161). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
10.1037/11877-008 Google Scholar
- Boulding, K. (1956). General systems theory—The skeleton of science. Management Science, 2, 197–208.
- Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.
- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.
- Bronfenbrenner, U., Kessel, F., Kessen, W., & White, S. (1986). Toward a critical social history of developmental psychology: A propaedeutic discussion. American Psychologist, 41, 1218–1230.
- Chandler, M. J., Lalonde, C. E., Sokol, B. W., & Hallett, D. (2003). Personal persistence, identity development, and suicide: A study of native and non-native North American adolescents. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(No. 273).
- Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 597–600.
-
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences ( 2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
10.1046/j.1526-4610.2001.111006343.x Google Scholar
- Cole, M. (2006). Culture and cognitive development in phylogenetic, historical, and ontogenetic perspective. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception and language ( 6th ed., pp. 636–683). New York: Wiley.
- Defries, G. E., & McLearn, J. C. (1973). Introduction to behavioral genetics. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.
- Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., et al. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents’ experiences ins schools and in families. American Psychologist, 48, 90–101.
- Elder, G. H., Jr. (1979). Historical change in life patterns and personality. In P. Baltes & O. Brim. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (pp. 117–159). New York: Academic Press.
-
Elder, G. H., Jr., Johnson, M. K., & Crosnoe, R. (2003). The emergence and development of life course theory. In
J. T. Mortimer &
M. J. Shanahan (Eds.), Handbook of the life course (pp. 3–19). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.
10.1007/978-0-306-48247-2_1 Google Scholar
- Eldredge, N., & Gould, S. J. (1972). Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In T. J. M. Schopf (Ed.), Models in paleobiology (pp. 82–115). San Francisco: Freeman Cooper.
- Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49(8), 725–747.
- Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Issues, 1, 1–171.
- Fiese, B. H., & Winter, M. A. (2009). The dynamics of family chaos and its relation to children’s socio-emotional well being. In G. W. Evans & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective (pp. 55–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Furstenberg, F. F., Jr., Cook, T. D., Eccles, J., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Sameroff, A. (1999). Managing to make it: Urban families and adolescent success. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Galton, F. (1876). The history of twins, as a criterion for the relative powers of nature and nurture. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 5, 391–406.
- Gelman, S. A. (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115–140.
- Gladwell, M. (2008). Outliers: The story of success. New York: Little, Brown.
- Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Gutman, L. M., Sameroff, A. J., & Cole, R. (2003). Academic growth curve trajectories from first to twelfth grades: Effects of multiple social risk and preschool child factors. Developmental Psychology, 39, 777–790.
- Hinde, R. A. (1997). Relationships: A dialectical perspective. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
- Huston, A. C. (2008). From research to policy and back. Child Development, 79, 1–12.
- Kessen, W. (1962). “Stage” and “structure” in the study of children. In W. Kessen & C. Kuhlman (Eds.), Thought in the young child: Report of a conference with particular attention to the work of Jean Piaget. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 27(No. 83).
- Kessen, W. (1979). The American child and other cultural inventions. American Psychologist, 34, 815–820.
- Lorenz, K. (1950). Innate behaviour patterns. In Symposia of the society for experimental biology no. 4: Physiological mechanisms in animal behaviour. New York: Academic Press.
- Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of their infant from mother’s representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused and abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 203–217.
- Maxwell, L. E. (2009). Chaos outside the home: The school environment. In G. W. Evans & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective (pp. 117–136). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
-
McKenzie, M. J., & McDonough, S. C. (2009). Transactions between perception and reality: Maternal beliefs and infant regulatory behavior. In
A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 35–54). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
10.1037/11877-003 Google Scholar
-
Morrison, F. J., & Connor, C. M. (2009). The transition to school: Child instruction transactions in learning to read. In
A. Sameroff (Ed.), The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other (pp. 183–201). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
10.1037/11877-010 Google Scholar
- Parke, R. D. (2004). The Society for Research in Child Development at 70: Progress and promise. Child Development, 75, 1–24.
- Patterson, G. R. (1986). Performance models for antisocial boys. American Psychologist, 41, 432–444.
- Princeton Review. (2009). Cracking the GRE. New York: Princeton Review Books.
- Raver, C. C. (2004). Placing emotional self-regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts. Child Development, 75, 346–353.
- Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American Psychologist, 31, 689–700.
- Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
- Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.
- Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In W. Kessen (Series Ed.) & P. H. Mussen (Vol Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. History, theories, and methods. (pp. 238–294). New York: Wiley.
- Sameroff, A. J. (1999). Ecological perspectives on developmental risk. In J. D. Osofsky & H. E. Fitzgerald (Eds.), WAIMH handbook of infant mental health: Vol. 4. Infant mental health groups at risk (pp. 223–248). New York: Wiley.
- Sameroff, A. J. (2006). Identifying risk and protective factors for healthy youth development. In A. Clarke-Stewart & J. Dunn (Eds.), Families count: Effects on child and adolescent development (pp. 53–76). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
- A. J. Sameroff (Ed.) (2009). The transactional model of development: How children and contexts shape each other. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
- Sameroff, A. J., & Chandler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In F. D. Horowitz, M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-Salapatek, & G. Siegel (Eds.), Review of child development research (Vol. 4, pp. 187–244). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- A. J. Sameroff, & R. N. Emde (Eds.) (1989). Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A developmental approach. New York: Basic Books.
- Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Transactional regulation: The developmental ecology of early intervention. In J. P. Shonkoff & S. J. Meisels (Eds.), Early intervention: A handbook of theory, practice, and analysis ( 2nd ed., pp. 135–159). New York: Cambridge University Press.
- A. J. Sameroff, & M. Haith (Eds.). (1996). The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Seifer, R., Sameroff, A. J., Barrett, L. C., & Krafchuk, E. (1994). Infant temperament measured by multiple observations and mother report. Child Development, 65, 1478–1490.
- Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.
- Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford.
- Taylor, M., & Carlson, S. M. (2009). The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind. Child Development, 68, 436–455.
- Thelen, E. (1989). Self-organization in developmental processes: Can systems approaches work? In M. R. Gunnar & E. Thelen (Eds.), Systems and development: The Minnesota symposia on child psychology (Vol. 22, pp. 77–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General system theory ( Rev. ed.). New York: Braziller.
-
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
10.1525/eth.1990.18.3.02a00020 Google Scholar
-
Watson, J. D. (1914). Behavior: An introduction to comparative psychology. New York: Holt.
10.1037/10868-000 Google Scholar
- Weisner, T. S. (2002). Ecocultural understanding of children’s developmental pathways. Human Development, 45, 275–281.
- Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International University Press.
- Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development (pp. 125–148). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
-
White, S. H. (1965). Evidence for a hierarchical arrangement of learning processes.
Advances in Child Behavior and Development, 2, 187–220.
10.1016/S0065-2407(08)60483-8 Google Scholar
- White, S. H. (1996). The child’s entry into the “Age of Reason.” In A. J. Sameroff & M. M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility (pp. 17–30). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Worthman, C. M. (1993). Biocultural interactions in human development. In M. E. Pereira & L. A. Banks (Eds.), Juvenile primates: Life history, development, and behavior (pp. 339–358). New York: Oxford University Press.